Misplaced Pages

Declarative memory: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from[REDACTED] with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 04:20, 20 April 2010 editAbductive (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers128,990 editsm Hippocampal lesion studies: typo← Previous edit Latest revision as of 08:47, 9 April 2017 edit undoPaine Ellsworth (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, Page movers, File movers, New page reviewers, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers, Template editors256,297 edits add rcats 
(136 intermediate revisions by 86 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
#REDIRECT ]
'''Declarative memory''' (sometimes referred to as ]) is one of two types of long term human ]. It refers to memories which can be consciously recalled such as facts and events.<ref>Ullman MT. Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 2004; 92: 231–70.</ref> Its counterpart is known as non-declarative or ], which refers to unconscious memories such as skills (e.g. learning to ride a bicycle). Declarative memory can be divided into two distinct categories: ] and ].<ref name = "tulving1">Tulving E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of Memory, ed. E Tulving, W Donaldson, pp. 381–403. New York: Academic</ref>


{{Redirect category shell|h=The redirect and target are very closely related but not quite the same.|
== Types of declarative memory ==
{{R from merge}}

{{R to related topic}}
There are two types of declarative memory:
{{R printworthy}}
;Semantic memory: Semantic memories are those that store general factual knowledge that is independent of personal experience. Examples include types of food, capital cities, lexical knowledge (vocabulary), etc.<ref name = "tulving1" />
}}

;Episodic memory: Episodic memories are those that store specific events such as attending a class or flying to France. It can be thought of as mentally reliving a past event. Episodic memory is believed by many to be the system that supports and underpins semantic memory.<ref name = "tulving1" />

==History==

The study of human memory stretches back over the last 2000 years. An early attempt to understand memory can be found in ] major treatise, ], in which he compares the human ] to a ] <ref>Aristotle, On the Soul (De Anima), in Aristotle, Volume 4, Loeb Classical Library, William Heinemann, London, UK, 1936.</ref>. He theorized that all humans are born free of any knowledge and are the sum of their experiences. It wasn’t until the late 1800’s, however, that a young German philosopher by the name of ] developed the first scientific approach to studying memory.<ref>Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. Teachers College, Columbia University</ref> While some of his findings have endured and remain relevant to this day (]), his greatest contribution to the field of memory research was demonstrating that memory can be studied scientifically. In 1972, ] proposed the distinction between episodic and semantic memory <ref name = "tulving1" />. This was quickly adopted and is now widely accepted. Following this, in 1985, ] proposed a more general distinction between explicit (declarative) and implicit (procedural) memory <ref>Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimen-tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 501-518.</ref> With the recent advances in ] technology, there have been a multitude of findings linking specific brain areas to declarative memory. Despite these advances in ], there is still much to be discovered in terms of the operating mechanisms of declarative memory <ref name = "eich4">Eichenbaum, Howard (1997). Declarative memory: Insights from cognitive neurobiology. Annual Review of Psychology. Vol 48, 547-572.</ref>. It is unclear whether declarative memory is mediated by a particular “memory system” or if it is more accurately classified as a “type of knowledge” and it is not known how or why declarative memory evolved to begin with <ref name = "eich4" />.

==Neuropsychology==
===Normal brain function===
====Hippocampus====
]
Although many psychologists believe that the entire brain is involved with memory, the '']'' and surrounding structures appear to be most important in declarative memory specifically<ref name = "eich2">Eichenbaum, Howard (2001). The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Codes. Behavioural Brain Research, Vol 127: 199-207.</ref>. The ability to retain and recall episodic memories is highly dependant on the hippocampus <ref name = "eich2" />, whereas the formation of new declarative memories relies on both the hippocampus and '']''<ref name = "gab1">Gabrieli, J., & Kao, Y. (2007). Development of the Declarative Memory System in the Human Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 10: 1198-1205.</ref> Other studies have found that the parahippocampal cortices were related to superior '']''<ref name = "gab1" />.

The Three Stage Model was developed by Eichenbaum, et. Al (2001), and proposes that the hippocampus does three things with episodic memory:
# Mediates the recording of episodic memories
# Identifies common features between episodes
# Links these common episodes in a memory space.
To support this model, a version of ] was used to show that the hippocampus is in fact used as the memory space<ref name = "eich2" />.

When experiencing an event for the first time, a link is formed in the hippocampus allowing us to recall that event in the future. Separate links are also made for features related to that event. For example, when you meet someone new, a unique link is created for them. More links are then connected to that person’s link so you can remember what colour their shirt was, what the weather was like when you met them, etc. Specific episodes are made easier to remember and recall by repeatedly exposing oneself to them (which strengthens the links in the memory space) allowing for faster retrieval when remembering<ref name = "eich2" />.

Hippocampal cells ('']'') are activated depending on what information one is exposed to at that moment. Some cells are specific to spatial information, certain stimuli (smells, etc.), or behaviours as has been shown in a ''Radial Maze Task''<ref name = "eich2" />. It is therefore the hippocampus that allows us to recognize certain situations, environments, etc. as being either distinct or similar to others. However, the Three Stage Model does not incorporate the importance of other cortical structures in memory.

====Prefrontal cortex====

The lateral ] (PFC) is essential for remembering contextual details of an experience rather than for memory formation<ref name = "gab1" />. The PFC is also more involved with episodic memory than semantic memory, although it does play a small role in semantics.<ref name = "graham1">Graham, S., Levine, B. (2004). The Fundamental Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 16: 1633-1646.</ref>

Using PET studies and word stimuli, ] found that remembering is an automatic process<ref name = "craik1">Craik, F.I.M., Houle, S. (1994). Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Human Episodic Memory: Lessons From PET Studies. Biol. Psychiatry, 42: 75S-76S.</ref>. It is also well documented that a hemispheric asymmetry occurs in the PFC: When encoding memories, the Left ] PFC (LPFC) is activated, and when retrieving memories, activation is seen in the Right Dorsolateral PFC (RPFC)<ref name = "craik1" />.

Studies have also shown that the PFC is extremely involved with autonoetic consciousness (See ])<ref name = "stuss1">Stuss, D.T., Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a Theory of Episodic Memory: The Frontal Lobes & Autonoetic Consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121: 331-354.</ref>.This is responsible for humans’ recollective experiences and ‘mental time travelling’ abilities (characteristics of episodic memory).

]

====Amygdala====
The ] is believed to be involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotionally charged memories. Much of the evidence for this has come from research on a phenomenon known as ] . These are instances in which memories of powerful emotional events are more highly detailed and enduring than regular memories (e.g. ], ]). These memories have been linked to increased activation in the amygdala.<ref>Sharot T, Martorella EA, Delgado MR, Phelps EA. How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:389–394.</ref> Recent studies of patients with damage to the amygdala suggest that it is involved in memory for general knowledge, and not for specific information.<ref>Adolphs R, Tranel D, Buchanan TW (2005) Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nat Neurosci 8:512–518</ref><ref>Adolphs R, Denburg NL, Tranel D (2001) The amygdala’s role in long-term declarative memory for gist and detail. Behav Neurosci 115:983–992</ref>

====Other structures involved====

The regions of the ] have shown brain activation when a remote memory is being recovered<ref name = "graham1" /> and the ], Ventral ], and ] all play a role in memory formation<ref name = "gab1" />.

===Lesion studies===

] studies are commonly used in cognitive neuroscience research. Lesions can occur naturally through trauma or disease, or they can be surgically induced by researchers. In the study of declarative memory, the hippocampus and the amygdala are two structures frequently examined using this technique.

====Hippocampal lesion studies====
{{wikify|date=April 2010}}
]
:;]

:: The Morris water navigation task is a task that tests spatial learning in rats.<ref name = "morris1">Eichenbaum, H., Stewart, C. & Morris, R. G. M. Hippocampal representation in spatial learning. J. Neurosci. 10, 331–339 (1990).</ref> In this test rats learn to escape from a pool by swimming toward a platform submerged just below the surface of the water. Visual cues that surround the pool (i.e. Chair or window) help the rat to locate the platform on subsequent trials. The rats' use of specific events, cues and places are all forms of declarative memory.<ref name = "howard1">Eichenbaum, H. (2000). a cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 41-50.</ref>

:: There are 2 groups of rats that are observed, a control group with no lesions and an experimental group possessing hippocampal lesions. In this task created by Morris, et al, rats are placed in the pool at the same position for 12 trials. Each trial is timed and the path taken by the rats is recorded. Rats with hippocampal lesions successfully learn to locate the platform. If the starting point is moved, the rats with hippocampal lesions typically fail to locate the platform. The control rats, however, are able to find the platform using the cues acquired during the learning trials.<ref name = "morris1" /> This demonstrates the involvement of the hippocampus in declarative memory.<ref name = "howard1" />

:;Odor-odor Recognition Task

::A study by Bunsey and Eichenbaum involved a social encounter task between two rats(a subject and a demonstrator). The demonstrator, after eating a specific type of food, interacts with the subject rat, who then smells the food odor on the others breath. The experimenters then presented the subject rat with a decision between two food options; the food previously eaten by the demonstrator, and a novel food. The researchers found that when there was no time delay, both control rats and rats with lesions chose the familiar food. After 24 hours, however, the rats with hippocampal lesions were just as likely to eat both types of food, while control rats chose the familiar food.<ref>Bunsey, M. & Eichenbaum, H. Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus-stimulus association. Hippocampus 5, 546–556 (1995).</ref> This can be attributed to the inability to form episodic memories due to lesions in the hippocampus. The effects of this study can be observed in humans with amnesia, indicating the role of the hippocampus in developing episodic memories that can be generalized to similar situations.<ref name = "howard1" />

:;]

::Patient H.M. had parts of his medial temporal lobe removed which resulted in the loss of the ability to form new memories.<ref name = "cork1">Corkin, S. (2002). Perspectives: What’s new with the amnesic patient H.M.?.Nature Reviews Neuroscience,3, 153-160.</ref> The long-term declarative memory was crucially affected when the structures from the medial temporal lobe were removed, including the ability to form of new semantic knowledge and memories.<ref>Gabrieli, J. D. E., Cohen, N. J. & Corkin, S. The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection. Brain Cogn. 7,157–177 (1988).</ref> The dissociation in H.M. between the acquisition of declarative memory and other kinds of learning was seen initially in motor learning.<ref>Corkin, S. Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision. Neuropsychologia 6, 225–264 (1968).</ref> Declarative memory, was not working in H.M’s memory function, this was seen when H.M. completed the task of repetition ]. His performance does improve over trials, however, his scores were inferior to those of control participants.<ref>Milner, B., Corkin, S. & Teuber, H.-L. Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M. Neuropsychologia 6, 215–234 (1968).</ref> In the condition of H.M. the same results from this priming task are reflected when looking at the other basic memory functions like remembering, recall and recognizing.<ref name = "cork1" /> Lesions should not be interpreted as and all or nothing condition, in the case of H.M. not all memory and recognition is lost, although the declarative memory is severely damaged he still has a sense of self and memories that were developed before the lesion occurred.<ref>Aggleton, J. P. & Brown, M. W. Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 425–489 (1999).</ref>

====Amygdala lesion studies====

:;Emotional Arousal Task

::Adolph, Cahill and Schul completed a study showing that emotional arousal facilitates the encoding of material into long term declarative memory.<ref name = "Adolphs">Adolphs, R., Cahill, L., Schul, R., & Babinsky, R. (1997). Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learning and Memory, 4, 291-300.</ref> They selected two subjects with ] damage to the amygdala, as well as six control subjects and six subjects with brain damage. All subjects were shown a series of twelve slides accompanied by a narrative. The slides varied in the degree to which they evoked emotion - slides 1 through 4 and slides 9 through 12 contain non-emotional content. Slides 5 through 8 contain emotional material, and the seventh slide contained the most emotionally arousing image and description (a picture of surgically repaired legs of a car crash victim).<ref name = "Adolphs" />

::The emotionally arousing slide (slide 7) was remembered no better by the bilateral damage participants than any of the other slides. All other participants notably remembered the seventh slide the best and in most detail out of all the other slides.<ref name = "Adolphs" /> This shows that the amygdala is necessary to facilitate encoding of declarative knowledge regarding emotionally arousing stimuli, but is not required for encoding knowledge of emotionally neutral stimuli.<ref>Babinsky, R., Calabrese, P., Durwen, H., Markowitsch, H., & Brechtelsbuauer, D. (1993). The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory. Behav. Neuro, 6, 167-170.</ref>

==Factors that affect declarative memory==
===Stress===

Stress has a very large impact on the formation of declarative memories. Lupien, et al. completed a study that had 3 phases for participants to take part in. Phase 1 involved memorizing a series of words, phase 2 entailed either a stressful (public speaking) or non-stressful situation (an attention task), and phase 3 required participants to recall the words they learned in phase 1. A declarative memory was formed in phase 1 if the words shown to participants were remembered. There were signs of decreased declarative memory performance in the participants that had to complete the stressful situation after learning the words. This showed that the stress of the situation impaired participants’ ability to form concrete declarative knowledge<ref>Lupien, S., Gaudreau, S., Tchiteya, B., Maheu, F., Sharma, S., Nair, N., et al. (1997). Stress-Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects: Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 82(7), 2070-2075.</ref>. In the non stressful situation, participants could easily remember the words learned from phase 1.

] (PTSD) emerges after exposure to a traumatic event eliciting fear, horror or helplessness that involves bodily injury, the threat of injury, or death to one’s self or another person<ref>Cabeza, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Publishing Group, 7, 54-64.</ref> The chronic stress in PTSD contributes to an observed decrease in hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits<ref>Baker, D. G. et al. Higher levels of basal CSF cortisol in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 992–994 (2005).</ref>.

====Neurochemical factors of stress on the brain====

In the brain, ] (GC's) modulate the ability of the hippocampus and PFC to process memories<ref name = "damoi1">Damoiseaux, J.S., Elzinga, B.M. (2007). Glucocorticoids Decrease Hippocampal and Prefrontal Activation during Declarative Memory Retrieval in Young Men. Brain Imaging and Behaviour, Vol 1: 31-41.</ref>. ] is one of the most common GC’s in the human body, and hydrocortisone (a derivative of cortisol) decreases brain activity in the above areas during declarative memory retrieval<ref name = "damoi1" />.

Elevations in cortisol occur during stress, and long-term stress impairs declarative memory this way<ref name = "damoi1" />. A study done by Damoiseaux, et. Al evaluated the effect of glucocorticoids on MTL and PFC activation in young men. They found that GC’s given to participants 1 hour before retrieval of information impairs free recall of words, yet when administered before or after learning they had no effect<ref name = "damoi1" />. Although it is not known exactly how GC’s influence memory, there are ]s in the hippocampus and PFC that tell us these structures are targets for the circulating hormone<ref name = "damoi1" />. However, it is known that cortisone impairs memory function by reducing the blood flow in the right parahippocampal gyrus, left visual cortex, and the ]<ref name = "damoi1" />.

Note: This study only involved male subjects which may be significant as sex steroids may have different effects in the responses to cortisol administration. Men and women also respond differently to emotional stimuli and this may affect cortisol levels. Also, this study was the first ](fMRI) study to be done involving GC's and more research is necessary to support these findings<ref name = "damoi1" />.

==Declarative memory consolidation during sleep==

It is believed by many researchers that ] plays an active role in consolidation of declarative memory. Specifically, sleep’s unique properties enhance '']'', such as the reactivation of newly learned memories during sleep. For example, it has been suggested that the central mechanism for consolidation of declarative memory during sleep is the reactivation of hippocampal memory representations. Specifically, this reactivation transfers information to neocortical networks where it is integrated into long-term representations <ref>McClelland, J.L., McNaughton, B.L., and O’Reilly, R.C. 1995. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102: 419–457.</ref>. For instance, studies on rats involving maze learning found that hippocampal neuronal assemblies that are used in the encoding of spatial information are reactivated in the same temporal order <ref>D. Ji, M. A. Wilson, Nat. Neurosci. 10, 100 (2007)</ref>. Similarly, positron emission tomography (PET) has shown reactivation of the '']'' in slow-wave sleep (SWS) after spatial learning <ref>P. Peigneux et al., Neuron 44, 535 (2004)</ref>. Together these studies show that newly learned memories are reactivated during sleep and through this, help consolidate new memory traces<ref name = "ellenb1">Ellenbogen J, Payne D, et al. (2006) The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2006, 16:716–722</ref>. In addition, researchers to date have pinpointed three types of sleep (SWS, Sleep Spindle and REM) which they believe contribute to declarative memory consolidation.

;SWS

:], often referred to as deep sleep, plays the most important role in consolidation of declarative memory and there is a large amount of evidence to support this claim. For instance, one study found that the first 3.5 hours of sleep offer the greatest performance enhancement on memory recall tasks, due to the fact that first couple of hours are dominated by SWS. Yet, additional hours of sleep do not add to the initial level of performance. Thus, this study suggests that full sleep may not be important for optimal performance of memory<ref>Tucker A, Fishbein W, (2009) The Impact of sleep duration and subject intelligence on declarative and motor memory performance: how much is enough? J. Sleep Res., 304-312</ref>. Another study shows that people who experience SWS during the first half of their sleep cycle compared to subjects who did not, showed better recall of information. However this is not the case for subjects who were tested for the second half of their sleep cycle, as they experience less SWS.<ref>Plihal W, Born J: Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cogn Neurosci 1997, 9:534-547.</ref>.

:Another key piece of evidence regarding SWS’s involvement in declarative memory consolidation is a finding that people with pathological conditions of sleep, such as insomnia, exhibit both reduction in '']'' and also have impaired consolidation of declarative memory during sleep<ref>Backhaus, J., Junghanns, K., Born, J., Hohaus, K., Faasch, F., and Hohagen, F. 2006. Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary insomnia: Influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release. Biol. Psychiatry 60: 1324–1330.</ref>. Yet, another study found that middle aged people compared to young group had a worse retrieval of memories. This in turn indicated that SWS is associated with poor declarative memory consolidation but not with age itself<ref>Backhaus J, Born J, et al. (2007) Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep. Learning & Memory, 14: 336-341</ref>. In sum, the above studies and numerous others demonstrate that SWS indeed plays a big role in consolidation of declarative memory.

;Sleep Spindle

: Some researchers believe that '']'', a burst of brain activity occurring during stage 2 sleep, plays a role in boosting consolidation of declarative memories <ref>Gais S, Molle M, Helms K, Born J: Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J Neurosci 2002, 22:6830-6834.</ref>. Some critics point out that spindle activity is positively correlated with intelligence.<ref>Schabus M, Hodlmoser K, Gruber G, Sauter C, Anderer P, Klosch G, Parapatics S, Saletu B, Klimesch W, Zeitlhofer J: Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to
general cognitive and learning abilities. Eur J Neurosci 2006, 23:1738-1746</ref>. Nevertheless, Schabus and Gruber point out that sleep-spindle activity only relates to performance on newly learned memories, but not to absolute performance. This supports the hypothesis that sleep spindle helps to consolidate recent memory traces but not memory performance in general <ref>Schabus M; Gruber G; Parapatics S et al. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. SLEEP 2004;27(8):1479-85</ref>.

More research is needed to fully understand the relationship between sleep spindles and declarative memory consolidation.

;REM

: There is a relatively small, yet growing body of evidence that supports the idea that '']'' helps consolidate highly emotional declarative memories. For instance Wagner, et al. compared memory retention for emotional versus neutral text over two instances; early sleep that is dominated by SWS and late sleep that is dominated by REM sleep<ref>Wagner U, Gais S, Born J: Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn Mem 2001, 8:112-119.</ref>. He found that sleep improved memory retention of emotional text only during late sleep phase, which was primarily dominated by REM sleep. Similarly Hu & Stylos-Allen, et al. did a study with emotional versus neutral pictures and came to the same conclusion that REM sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional declarative memories<ref>Hu P, Stylos-Allen M, Walker MP: Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing declarative memory. Psychol Sci 2006, 17, in press.</ref>.

However, the view that sleep play’s an active role in declarative memory consolidation is not shared by all of the researchers. For instance Ellenbogen, et al.<ref>Ellenbogen J; Hulbert J; et al. Interfering with Theories of Sleep and Memory: Sleep, Declarative Memory, and Associative Interference. CURRENT BIOLOGY 2006; 16 1290-1294.</ref> argues that sleep actively protects declarative memory from associative interference. Furthermore, Wixted believes that the sole role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation is nothing more but creating ideal conditions for memory consolidation<ref>Wixted JT: The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annu Rev Psychol 2004, 55:235-269.</ref>. For example, when awake, people are bombarded with mental activity which interferes with effective consolidation. However, during sleep, when interference is minimal, memories can be consolidated without any obstacles. In sum, this view suggests that sleep provides ideal conditions for declarative memory consolidation but does not actively enhance memory consolidation. However more research is needed to make a definite statement whether sleep creates favourable conditions for consolidation or it actively enhances declarative memory consolidation<ref name = "ellenb1" />.

==In the media==
]cs are frequently portrayed in television and movies. Some of the better known examples include:

]

;]

:In the popular romantic comedy 50 First Dates (2004), Adam Sandler plays veterinarian Henry Roth, who falls for Lucy Whitmore (played by Drew Barrymore). The Character Lucy had lost her short term memory in a car crash, and she can only remember the current days events until she falls asleep. When she wakes up the next morning, she has no recollection of the previous days experiences.<ref name="Baxtendale, S. 2004">Baxtendale, S. (2004). Memories aren’t made of this: Amnesia at the movies. British Medical Journal. 329: 1480-1483</ref> These experiences would normally be transferred into declarative knowledge, allowing them to be recalled in the future. Although this movie is not the most accurate representation of a true amnesic patient, it is useful for informing viewers of the detremental effects of amnesia.

;]

:Memento (2000) a film inspired by the case of Henry Molaison (H.M.).<ref>Chun, M. (2005). Drug-induced amnesia impairs implicit relational memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(8), 355-357.</ref> ] plays an ex-insurance investigator suffering from severe ] caused by a head injury. Unlike most amnesiacs, Leonard retains his identity and the memories of events that occurred before the injury, but loses all ability to form new memories. This loss of ability to form new memories indicates that the head injury effected the medial temporal lobe of the brain resulting in the inability for Leonard to form declarative memory.

;]

:Finding Nemo features a ] with an inability to develop declarative memory. This prevents her from learning or retaining any new information such as names or directions. The exact origin of Dory's impairment is not mentioned in the film, but her memory loss accurately portrays the difficulties facing amnesiacs.<ref name="Baxtendale, S. 2004"/>

==References==

{{reflist}}
{{memory}}

]

]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 08:47, 9 April 2017

Redirect to:

This page is a redirect. The following categories are used to track and monitor this redirect:The redirect and target are very closely related but not quite the same.
  • From a merge: This is a redirect from a page that was merged into another page. This redirect was kept in order to preserve the edit history of this page after its content was merged into the content of the target page. Please do not remove the tag that generates this text (unless the need to recreate content on this page has been demonstrated) or delete this page.
  • To a related topic: This is a redirect to an article about a similar topic.
    • Redirects from related topics are different than redirects from related words, because a related topic is more likely to warrant a full and detailed description in the target article. If this redirect's subject is notable, then also tag it with {{R with possibilities}} and {{R printworthy}}.
When appropriate, protection levels are automatically sensed, described and categorized.
Declarative memory: Difference between revisions Add topic