Revision as of 23:52, 9 March 2013 edit-sche (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers13,513 edits why not specify 'vowel and consonant' in the order they appear in the sequence, rather than in reverse?← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 14:17, 10 December 2024 edit undoLindeby (talk | contribs)138 editsm It's Daniel Jones, of courseTag: 2017 wikitext editor | ||
(41 intermediate revisions by 31 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{Short description|Distinguishing feature of a sound system by vowel or consonant length}} | |||
{{ |
{{more citations needed|date=February 2011}} | ||
In linguistics, a '''chroneme''' is a basic, theoretical unit of sound that can distinguish words by duration only of a vowel or consonant. The noun ''chroneme'' is derived from Greek χρονος (chronos, ''time''), and the suffixed ''-eme'', which is analogous to the ''-eme'' in '']'' or '']''. However, this term does not have wide currency, and may even be unknown to phonologists who work on languages claimed to have chronemes. | |||
{{IPA notice}} | |||
In linguistics, a '''chroneme''' is an abstract ] suprasegmental feature used to signify contrastive differences in the length of speech sounds. Both consonants and vowels can be viewed as displaying this features.<ref>{{Cite book |title=A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics |publisher=] |year=2008 |isbn=978-1-405-15296-9 |editor-last=Crystal |editor-first=David |edition=6th |series=The language library |pages=76 |language=en}}</ref> The noun ''chroneme'' is derived {{etymology|grc|''{{wikt-lang|grc|χρόνος}}'' ({{grc-transl|χρόνος}})|time}}, and the suffixed ''-eme'', which is analogous to the ''-eme'' in '']'' or '']''. Two words with different meaning that are spoken exactly the same except for length of one segment are considered a ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Jones |first=Daniel |date=1944 |title=Chronemes and tonemes: (a contribution to the study of the theory of phonemes) |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/03740463.1944.10410902 |journal=Acta Linguistica |language=en |volume=4 |issue=1 |pages=11–10 |doi=10.1080/03740463.1944.10410902 |issn=0105-001X}}</ref> The term was coined by the British phonetician ] to avoid using the term ''phoneme'' to characterize a feature above the segmental level.<ref>{{Cite book |editor-last=Brown |editor-first= Keith |last=Barry |first=William J. |title=Encyclopedia of language & linguistics |chapter=The Phoneme |date=2006 |publisher=Elsevier |isbn=978-0-08-044854-1 |edition=2nd |location=Amsterdam}}</ref> | |||
⚫ | |||
The term is not widely used today, and in the case of English phonetics, Jones' analysis of long and short vowels (e.g. the {{IPA|/iː/}} of ''bead'' and the {{IPA|/ɪ/}} of ''bit'' ) as distinguished only by the chroneme is now described as "no longer tenable".<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gimson |first=A.C. |date=1977-04-01 |title=Daniel Jones and standards of English pronunciation |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/00138387708597818 |journal=English Studies |volume=58 |issue=2 |pages=151-158 |doi=10.1080/00138387708597818 |issn=0013-838X}}</ref> | |||
For the purposes of analysis of a chronemic contrast, two words with different meaning that are spoken exactly the same except for length of one segment are considered a ]. | |||
⚫ | Languages can have differences in ] of vowels or consonants, but in most of them these differences aren not used phonemically or phonologically as distinctive or contrastive. Even in those languages which do have phonologically contrastive length, a chroneme is only posited in particular languages. Use of a chroneme views {{IPA|/aː/}} as being composed of two ]: {{IPA|/a/}} and {{IPA|/ː/}}, whereas in a particular analysis, {{IPA|/aː/}} may be considered a single segment with length being one of its ]. This may be compared to the analysis of a ] like {{IPA|}} as a single segment {{IPA|/ai/}} or as the sequence of a vowel and consonant: {{IPA|/aj/}}. | ||
The ] (IPA) denotes length by doubling the letter or by diacritics above or after the letters: | The ] (IPA) denotes length by doubling the letter or by diacritics above or after the letters: | ||
Line 11: | Line 14: | ||
! symbol || position || meaning | ! symbol || position || meaning | ||
|- | |- | ||
| style=" |
| style="text-align:center;" |''none'' || - || short | ||
|- | |- | ||
| style="font-size:larger;text-align:center;" | {{IPA|ː}} || after || long | | style="font-size:larger;text-align:center;" | {{IPA|ː}} || after || long | ||
Line 20: | Line 23: | ||
|} | |} | ||
==By languages== | |||
===English=== | |||
American English does not have minimal pairs indicating the existence of chronemes or may theoretically be said to have only one chroneme. Some other dialects such as ] have contrastive vowel length, but it is not analysed as the consequence of a chroneme. | American English does not have minimal pairs indicating the existence of chronemes or may theoretically be said to have only one chroneme. Some other dialects such as ] have contrastive vowel length, but it is not analysed as the consequence of a chroneme. | ||
Many ], including Classical ] have distinctive length in consonants |
=== Other Indo-European languages=== | ||
Many ], including Classical ], have distinctive length in consonants. For example, in ]: | |||
{|class="wikitable" | {|class="wikitable" | ||
Line 32: | Line 38: | ||
|} | |} | ||
or ]: | |||
⚫ | |||
{|class="wikitable" | {|class="wikitable" | ||
! |
! ]|| IPA || Quality || Etymology || English | ||
|- | |- | ||
| |
| A sicunna || {{IPA|/ˌa siˈkunna/}} || short || ''(ill)a(m)'' ("that/her") || the second ] one | ||
|- | |- | ||
| |
| A sicunna|| {{IPA|/ˌassiˈkunna/}} || long || ''a(d)'' ("at/to/etc.") || Depending on | ||
|} | |} | ||
Distinctive length in vowels may be presented by the cŭ + cū ] in the dialect spoken near ] (Italy): | |||
⚫ | Almost all ], such as ], ] and ] have a distinctive ] chroneme as a ] (also arguably called ] or ]/consonant). The etymology of the vocalic chroneme has been traced to a ] in the hypothetical ] language, such that {{IPA|}} becomes {{IPA|}}. For example, ''taka-'' "back-", ''takka'' "fireplace" and ''taakka'' "burden" are unrelated words. It is also grammatically important; the third person marker is a chroneme (''menee'' "s/he goes"), and often in the ] of the Helsinki area there are grammatical minimal pairs, e.g. nominative ''Stadi'' "Helsinki" vs. partitive ''Stadii'' |
||
{|class="wikitable" | |||
⚫ | In Finnish, Estonian and ], there are also two ] lengths of the chroneme, ''half-long'' and ''over-long''. For example, Finnish imperative ''ann'''a'''!'' "give!" has a short vowel, ''om'''a''''' "own" has a half-long vowel, and ''Ann'''aa''''' |
||
! ] spoken in Palmi || IPA || Quality || Etymology || Latin || English | |||
|- | |||
| Cu' voli? || {{IPA|/kuˈvɔːlɪ/}} || short || cŭ < lat. ''qui(s)'' ("who") || Quis vult? || Who wants? | |||
|- | |||
| Cu' u voli? || {{IPA|/kuːˈvɔːlɪ/}} || long || cū < lat. ''qui(s) (ill)ŭ(m)'' ("what/him") || Quis illum vult? || Who wants him/it? | |||
|} | |||
===Uralic languages=== | |||
⚫ | Almost all ], such as ], ] and ] have a distinctive ] chroneme as a ] (also arguably called ] or ]/consonant). The etymology of the vocalic chroneme has been traced to a ] in the hypothetical ] language, such that {{IPA|}} becomes {{IPA|}}. For example, ''taka-'' "back-", ''takka'' "fireplace" and ''taakka'' "burden" are unrelated words. It is also grammatically important; the third person marker is a chroneme (''menee'' "s/he goes"), and often in the ] of the Helsinki area there are grammatical minimal pairs, e.g. nominative ''Stadi'' "Helsinki" vs. partitive ''Stadii''. | ||
⚫ | In Finnish, Estonian and ], there are also two ] lengths of the chroneme, ''half-long'' and ''over-long''. For example, Finnish imperative ''ann'''a'''!'' "give!" has a short vowel, ''om'''a''''' "own" has a half-long vowel, and ''Ann'''aa''''' (partitive case of the name Anna) has an overlong vowel (without any distinctive tonal variation to distinguish these three). Estonian and Sami also have a three-way distinction in consonants, e.g. ''li'''n'''a'' "bed sheet", ''li'''nn'''a'' (half-long 'n') "of the city", ''li'''nn'''a'' (over-long 'n') "to the city". Estonian, in which the phonemic opposition is the strongest, uses tonal contour as a secondary cue to distinguish the two; "over-long" is falling as in other Finnic languages, but "half-long" is rising. | ||
Finnish also denotes stress principally by adding more length ( |
Finnish also denotes ] principally by adding more length (approximately 100 ms) to the vowel of the syllable nucleus. This means that Finnish has five different physical lengths. (The half-long vowel is a phonemically short vowel appearing in the second syllable, if the first—and thus stressed—syllable is a single short vowel.) The unstressed short vowels are about 40 ms in physical duration, the unstressed long vowels about 70 ms. The stress adds about 100 ms, giving short stressed as 130–150 ms and long stressed as 170–180 ms. The half-long vowel, which is always short unstressed, is distinctively longer than the standard 40 ms. | ||
===Japanese=== | |||
] is another language in which vowel length is distinctive. For example, ''biru'' is a foreign loan word (clipped from a longer form) that means |
] is another language in which vowel length is distinctive. For example, ''biru'' is a foreign loan word (clipped from a longer form) that means "building" whereas ''bīru'' is a foreign loan word for "beer". Using a notion intuitive to a speaker of Japanese, it could be said that more than anything, what differentiates ''bīru'' from ''biru'' is an extra mora (or minimal vowel syllable) in the speech rhythm that signifies a lengthening of the vowel {{IPA|}}. However, upon observation one might also note a rise in pitch and intensity of the longer vowel. It could be said, also, that vowel lengthening—chronemic contrasts—nearly doubles Japanese's rather small inventory of vowel phonemes (though the occurrence of diphthongs also augments vowel counts). Due to native literacy practices, Japanese long vowels are often thought of as sequences of two vowels of the same quality (rather than one vowel of a greater quantity or length) since that is how they are sometimes written. | ||
In the case of consonants of Japanese, if treated phonemically, a medial consonant might appear to double, thus creating a contrast, for example, between the word ''hiki'' (meaning 'pull' or 'influence') and ''hikki'' (meaning 'writing'). In terms of articulation and phonetics, the difference between the two words would be that, in the latter ''hikki'', the doubled {{IPA|}} closes the first syllable {{IPA|}} and is realized in the glottis as glottal plosive stop (with some anticipatory articulation evident in the velum of the mouth, where a {{IPA|/k/}} is usually made) while starting the next syllable {{IPA|}} as a {{IPA|}} articulated and realized as the regular velar sound. In effect, this consonant doubling then adds one mora to the overall speech rhythm and timing. Hence, among other contrasts, the word ''hik-ki'' is felt to be one mora or beat longer than ''hi-ki'' by a speaker of Japanese. <!--Isn't this essentially the same as Italian?--> | In the case of consonants of Japanese, if treated phonemically, a medial consonant might appear to double, thus creating a contrast, for example, between the word ''hiki'' (meaning 'pull' or 'influence') and ''hikki'' (meaning 'writing'). In terms of articulation and phonetics, the difference between the two words would be that, in the latter ''hikki'', the doubled {{IPA|}} closes the first syllable {{IPA|}} and is realized in the glottis as glottal plosive stop (with some anticipatory articulation evident in the velum of the mouth, where a {{IPA|/k/}} is usually made) while starting the next syllable {{IPA|}} as a {{IPA|}} articulated and realized as the regular velar sound. In effect, this consonant doubling then adds one mora to the overall speech rhythm and timing. Hence, among other contrasts, the word ''hik-ki'' is felt to be one mora or beat longer than ''hi-ki'' by a speaker of Japanese. <!--Isn't this essentially the same as Italian?--> | ||
===Thai=== | |||
⚫ | ] has distinctive length in ]s. For example: | ||
{|class="wikitable" | |||
! word || IPA ||]|| quality || meaning | |||
|- | |||
| เข้า || {{IPA|/kʰâw/}} || khâo || short || enter | |||
|- | |||
| ข้าว || {{IPA|/kʰâːw/}} || khâo || long || rice | |||
|} | |||
==See also== | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
==References== | ==References== | ||
{{ |
{{Reflist}} | ||
⚫ | *Suomi, Kari. ''Temporal conspiracies for a tonal end: Segmental durations and accentual f0 movement in a quantity language.'' Journal of Phonetics, Volume 33, Issue 3, July 2005, |
||
⚫ | * | ||
{{refend}} | |||
==Bibliography== | |||
⚫ | ] | ||
⚫ | *]. ''Temporal conspiracies for a tonal end: Segmental durations and accentual f0 movement in a quantity language.'' Journal of Phonetics, Volume 33, Issue 3, July 2005, pp. 291–309. | ||
⚫ | * | ||
==External links== | |||
* {{wiktionary-inline}} | |||
{{Suprasegmentals}} | {{Suprasegmentals}} | ||
{{wikt}} | |||
⚫ | ] |
Latest revision as of 14:17, 10 December 2024
Distinguishing feature of a sound system by vowel or consonant lengthThis article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. Find sources: "Chroneme" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (February 2011) (Learn how and when to remove this message) |
In linguistics, a chroneme is an abstract phonological suprasegmental feature used to signify contrastive differences in the length of speech sounds. Both consonants and vowels can be viewed as displaying this features. The noun chroneme is derived from Ancient Greek χρόνος (khrónos) 'time', and the suffixed -eme, which is analogous to the -eme in phoneme or morpheme. Two words with different meaning that are spoken exactly the same except for length of one segment are considered a minimal pair. The term was coined by the British phonetician Daniel Jones to avoid using the term phoneme to characterize a feature above the segmental level.
The term is not widely used today, and in the case of English phonetics, Jones' analysis of long and short vowels (e.g. the /iː/ of bead and the /ɪ/ of bit ) as distinguished only by the chroneme is now described as "no longer tenable".
Languages can have differences in length of vowels or consonants, but in most of them these differences aren not used phonemically or phonologically as distinctive or contrastive. Even in those languages which do have phonologically contrastive length, a chroneme is only posited in particular languages. Use of a chroneme views /aː/ as being composed of two segments: /a/ and /ː/, whereas in a particular analysis, /aː/ may be considered a single segment with length being one of its features. This may be compared to the analysis of a diphthong like as a single segment /ai/ or as the sequence of a vowel and consonant: /aj/.
The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) denotes length by doubling the letter or by diacritics above or after the letters:
symbol | position | meaning |
---|---|---|
none | - | short |
ː | after | long |
ˑ | after | half-long |
˘ | above | extra-short |
By languages
English
American English does not have minimal pairs indicating the existence of chronemes or may theoretically be said to have only one chroneme. Some other dialects such as Australian English have contrastive vowel length, but it is not analysed as the consequence of a chroneme.
Other Indo-European languages
Many Indo-European languages, including Classical Latin, have distinctive length in consonants. For example, in Italian:
word | IPA | meaning |
---|---|---|
vile | /ˈvile/ | coward |
ville | /ˈville/ | villas |
or Sicilian:
Sicilian language | IPA | Quality | Etymology | English |
---|---|---|---|---|
A sicunna | /ˌa siˈkunna/ | short | (ill)a(m) ("that/her") | the second feminine one |
A sicunna | /ˌassiˈkunna/ | long | a(d) ("at/to/etc.") | Depending on |
Distinctive length in vowels may be presented by the cŭ + cū minimal pair in the dialect spoken near Palmi, Calabria (Italy):
Dialect spoken in Palmi | IPA | Quality | Etymology | Latin | English |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cu' voli? | /kuˈvɔːlɪ/ | short | cŭ < lat. qui(s) ("who") | Quis vult? | Who wants? |
Cu' u voli? | /kuːˈvɔːlɪ/ | long | cū < lat. qui(s) (ill)ŭ(m) ("what/him") | Quis illum vult? | Who wants him/it? |
Uralic languages
Almost all Uralic languages, such as Finnish, Hungarian and Estonian have a distinctive moraic chroneme as a phoneme (also arguably called archiphoneme or epenthetic vowel/consonant). The etymology of the vocalic chroneme has been traced to a voiced velar fricative in the hypothetical Proto-Uralic language, such that becomes . For example, taka- "back-", takka "fireplace" and taakka "burden" are unrelated words. It is also grammatically important; the third person marker is a chroneme (menee "s/he goes"), and often in the spoken Finnish of the Helsinki area there are grammatical minimal pairs, e.g. nominative Stadi "Helsinki" vs. partitive Stadii.
In Finnish, Estonian and Sami languages, there are also two allophonic lengths of the chroneme, half-long and over-long. For example, Finnish imperative anna! "give!" has a short vowel, oma "own" has a half-long vowel, and Annaa (partitive case of the name Anna) has an overlong vowel (without any distinctive tonal variation to distinguish these three). Estonian and Sami also have a three-way distinction in consonants, e.g. lina "bed sheet", linna (half-long 'n') "of the city", linna (over-long 'n') "to the city". Estonian, in which the phonemic opposition is the strongest, uses tonal contour as a secondary cue to distinguish the two; "over-long" is falling as in other Finnic languages, but "half-long" is rising.
Finnish also denotes stress principally by adding more length (approximately 100 ms) to the vowel of the syllable nucleus. This means that Finnish has five different physical lengths. (The half-long vowel is a phonemically short vowel appearing in the second syllable, if the first—and thus stressed—syllable is a single short vowel.) The unstressed short vowels are about 40 ms in physical duration, the unstressed long vowels about 70 ms. The stress adds about 100 ms, giving short stressed as 130–150 ms and long stressed as 170–180 ms. The half-long vowel, which is always short unstressed, is distinctively longer than the standard 40 ms.
Japanese
Japanese is another language in which vowel length is distinctive. For example, biru is a foreign loan word (clipped from a longer form) that means "building" whereas bīru is a foreign loan word for "beer". Using a notion intuitive to a speaker of Japanese, it could be said that more than anything, what differentiates bīru from biru is an extra mora (or minimal vowel syllable) in the speech rhythm that signifies a lengthening of the vowel . However, upon observation one might also note a rise in pitch and intensity of the longer vowel. It could be said, also, that vowel lengthening—chronemic contrasts—nearly doubles Japanese's rather small inventory of vowel phonemes (though the occurrence of diphthongs also augments vowel counts). Due to native literacy practices, Japanese long vowels are often thought of as sequences of two vowels of the same quality (rather than one vowel of a greater quantity or length) since that is how they are sometimes written.
In the case of consonants of Japanese, if treated phonemically, a medial consonant might appear to double, thus creating a contrast, for example, between the word hiki (meaning 'pull' or 'influence') and hikki (meaning 'writing'). In terms of articulation and phonetics, the difference between the two words would be that, in the latter hikki, the doubled closes the first syllable and is realized in the glottis as glottal plosive stop (with some anticipatory articulation evident in the velum of the mouth, where a /k/ is usually made) while starting the next syllable as a articulated and realized as the regular velar sound. In effect, this consonant doubling then adds one mora to the overall speech rhythm and timing. Hence, among other contrasts, the word hik-ki is felt to be one mora or beat longer than hi-ki by a speaker of Japanese.
Thai
Thai has distinctive length in vowels. For example:
word | IPA | RTGS | quality | meaning |
---|---|---|---|---|
เข้า | /kʰâw/ | khâo | short | enter |
ข้าว | /kʰâːw/ | khâo | long | rice |
See also
References
- Crystal, David, ed. (2008). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. The language library (6th ed.). Blackwell Publishing. p. 76. ISBN 978-1-405-15296-9.
- Jones, Daniel (1944). "Chronemes and tonemes: (a contribution to the study of the theory of phonemes)". Acta Linguistica. 4 (1): 11–10. doi:10.1080/03740463.1944.10410902. ISSN 0105-001X.
- Barry, William J. (2006). "The Phoneme". In Brown, Keith (ed.). Encyclopedia of language & linguistics (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-08-044854-1.
- Gimson, A.C. (1977-04-01). "Daniel Jones and standards of English pronunciation". English Studies. 58 (2): 151–158. doi:10.1080/00138387708597818. ISSN 0013-838X.
Bibliography
- Suomi, Kari. Temporal conspiracies for a tonal end: Segmental durations and accentual f0 movement in a quantity language. Journal of Phonetics, Volume 33, Issue 3, July 2005, pp. 291–309.
- Phonetics of Finnish: Quantity and duration of vowels & consonants
External links
- [REDACTED] The dictionary definition of chroneme at Wiktionary
Suprasegmentals | |
---|---|
Timing | |
Tone | |
Stress | |
Length | |
Prosody |